danivon wrote:Actually, I do. it is called "observer bias".
If you wish. Others call it "experience."
However, criminals are not separate from the rest of humanity, they are people. The difference is in behaviour, but it is not necessarily a permanent state of mind. There are those who are habitual criminals, and maybe you can claim these have a "criminal mindset". But there are aslo reformed criminals. And there are people who commit a criminal act once for all kinds of reasons and when those reasons go away do not do so again. Or who gain an extra reason (living through the repercussions of their crime, for example) to not do so again.
This is, sadly, not as many as we would like to see.
But even so, your observation of criminals misses the point that people who don't commit crime (including those who did before and have stopped), are not going to be convicted.
Right. How odd for me not to take into account those who don't commit crime.
So you are only seeing one side of the "failure" of the deterrent effect, and not those who actually are deterred.
Immeasurable and unknowable.
The question then is this:
We are talking about the death penalty, and your response is that prison is not a deterrence. OK. So can you show me that the death penalty is a deterrence?
And further, can you show that it is more of one than life imprisonment?
I don't need to show it because I've not made either argument. I'm indifferent. The death penalty
ought to be applied in some situations because it is the only means of justice.
That would take some comparative data. Not subjective application of your cod-psychological assessment of a subset of people - a subset that let it be clear, your work was all about being in opposition to, and so there is another possible source of unconscious bias at play.
Absolutely wrong. If I were "all about being in opposition" to criminals, I would never understand them. Empathy, in some measure, is an absolute necessity. One must listen and at least try to understand the reasoning.
And the discussion that we had which bbauska joined in on was observing that there has overall been a fall in crime (and murder) regardless of whether a death penalty was in place. Which means that whatever it is that is stopping criminal acts, it seems unlikely that it is the prospect of death penalties. Even if "common sense" tells you that it should be a significant factor, the data suggests that it is not.
Again, this is erring in terms of there being a connection between the two. Again, I don't care about the deterrence value of the death penalty. Some people are so evil that a visit from God Himself would not change their course.
I will leave you with this. If you want to argue that prison can't be much of a deterrent because criminals still commit crime, then the death penalty is also failing because in Connecticut and in Georgia the death penalty was in force at the time of the crimes of murder we have been looking at.
The death penalty, if actually carried out in a timely manner, might be a deterrent. However, we have no way of knowing because our system has become so convoluted.
That said, to compare prison life to the death penalty is a tough road. Criminals are not afraid of prison, but they might not have the same low regard for death--if they knew it was not decades away.
Connecticut reinstated the death penalty in 1973, and had last executed someone in 2005, before the Cheshire murders in 2007. The death penalty was abolished only this year (in 2012 it was abolished but not for those already on death row).
Georgia reinstated the death penalty in 1976. Between 1983 and 1997 (ie: before Gissendaner's crime), the state had executed 22 people.
Great. You've proven that the death penalty, rarely applied and then only after many years of appeal, is not necessarily a deterrent. So what? I've not argued that it is.
However, if it is applied, we know the recidivism rate is extremely low. Every study shows that.